Followers

Search This Blog

Tuesday, December 19, 2023

Book Review-Humanly Possible: Seven Hundred Years of Humanist Freethinking, Inquiry, and Hope By Sarah Bakewell

Sarah Bakewell amply demonstrated her scholarship in historical research and her storytelling skills in her previous two books: How to Live: Or a Life of Montaigne in One Question and Twenty Attempts at an Answer and At the Existentialist Café. Those two books made her one of my must-read authors, no matter the subject. Which is how I came to procure a copy of Humanly Possible. I would have gladly added this book to my To-Be-Read pile even without having experienced her writing, as I have been curious about the Humanists, or Freethinkers throughout my spiritual life. The topic had been covered previously in  Susan Jacoby’s Freethinker’s: A History of American Secularism. Bakewell’s tome is broader in scope as she delves into 700 years of humanist thoughts and philosophy starting from the earliest western thought rather than just covering the American version of Humanism.

Bakewell structured her book chronologically, carefully tracking the evolution of Humanist thoughts from the first stirrings of the human centered belief as a reaction against reliance on the supernatural. She lays out a step-by-step evolutionary history of Humanism, tracing the many threads of Humanist thought throughout western human history. Her exceptional story telling prowess is the glue that holds the narrative together, as the development and evolution of the Humanist oeuvre is not only heterogeneous, broad in scope, but also complicated. This approach could have resulted in overwhelming pedantry, putting the reader into a rut of repetitious time keeping and list making; but Bakewell’s way with a narrative saved the book.

Her ease with the convoluted philosophical developments of Humanism throughout history and most importantly, her analysis of the developments within their specific context and in a broader historical sense told a clear story and her expository rigor made the ideas become as alive as the narrative.

She started her story with Petrarch and Boccacio and ended with her statement of her own thoughts regarding the future of Humanism. The last chapter is where she most clearly expressed her own Humanist beliefs and her struggles with the realities of Humanism.

In between the first and last chapters, Bakewell laid out the salient histories and personalities of the historical figures which kept the ideas of Humanism alive. She also adds a nice touch in giving the readers a short list and description of all the historical figures that she will be introducing at the beginning of each chapter, which gives us a hint of what to expect.

The stories get more fully fleshed out as the chronology gets closer to the present time, as expected, but Bakewell clearly lays out the reasons why the historical figures are considered important to the story, and why they believed in what they believed in, giving the readers an understanding of  the motivation for each historical figure.

Such historically significant figures as Erasmus, Montaigne, Voltaire, Diderot, Hume, Paine, Bentham, Wilde, Frederick Douglas, E. F. Forster, Wilhelm Von Humboldt, John Stuart Mill, Charles Darwin, Thomas Henry Huxley, Robert G Ingersoll, Bertrand Russell, and Thomas Mann were all mentioned, their contributions to Humanism analyzed and expounded upon to give depth and breadth to the Humanist story.

Bakewell employed a chronological structure to tell the story, which initially felt constraining and as if the narrative of the early history of Humanism was rushed; to be fair, there are scant historical records for Bakewell to dig into. It wasn’t until later in the chronology of the history that the pace of the book settled into a steady and more attractive pace.

The chapters on Wilhelm Von Humboldt, John Stuart Mills, and Forster were the most riveting for me. Although the chapters that dealt with the roles that Erasmus, Montaigne, Hume, Wilde and Bentham played earlier in the narrative also readily captured the reader’s attention. The introduction of Darwin and Huxley historically shook up Humanism much as it did literarily to  this book, giving a welcomed dose of scientific inquiries into what had been a purely philosophical analysis. Robert Ingersoll was a figure that I had known about but had not read much about, I was glad that Bakewell gave me a dip into his thoughts.

Bertrand Russell, as always, played the intellectual elephant in the room as his long-lasting intellectual legacy dominated the latter conversations about Humanism, as his long and productive life coupled with his robust intellectual expositions dominated the conversations about Humanism during the 20th century, as with many other philosophical discussions.

It was a bravura work of historical investigation about a complicated and convoluted subject. My own dips into the Humanism beliefs filled me with hope for humankind but also left me struggling with contradictions, as any worthy system of belief should. Bakewell stated that she is dedicated to the Humanist combination of Freethinking, Inquiry, and Hope as her guiding principles, and I feel the same way; after all, who in their right minds can argue with those three principles.

As with all broad- and broad-minded beliefs, there are enough variations in beliefs and thought in addition to the copious amount of interpretations to dampen any hopes of unifying and standardizing the beliefs. As with all beliefs, even though Humanism has at this time distanced itself from the overtly supernatural frame of reference; yet humans have always desired a set of written and definitive principles around which any movement or system of belief can pivot, which is where the monotheistic religions are advantaged: it is easier to attribute all the belief to a single supernatural entity than unifying disparate principles.

Much as Christians had employed the group sourced King James Bible and the earlier Council of Nicea, there has been attempts at unifying Humanist principles, first to counter the blasphemy laws around the world; as having a set of human based ethics and scruples is seen as a challenge to the churches and religions as they exist today; and second to integrate all the beliefs that the Humanist claim to be central to defining Humanism. Bakewell included the Declaration of Modern Humanism as written by consensus in 2022 by the Humanists International in the appendix. In and of itself, the declaration is a fine document, it does indeed encompass much of what most believe Humanism holds to be important. Although the wordsmithing and qualifying compositions reflect the amount and characteristics of the compromising that went on behind the scenes when the declaration was written. While I understand that wordsmithing is necessary and even critical to the process of gaining acceptance from all the Humanists; it reflects the nature of the negotiations. Compromising negatively impacts the power of the system of belief, which then ameliorates the passions of the potential believers.

This is what I find to be frustrating with Humanism. A belief system that is as admirable in its intentions and motivation as Humanism deserves serious consideration as a belief system, yet the main reason that it so attractive: the humanity focused belief as opposed to the supernatural belief, handicaps the clarity and unity of the Humanist philosophy. Being able to focus on the supernatural and having a centralized power and bureaucratic structure based on the belief of a monolithic idol creates a unity that is very powerful, even though the truth of the existence of the idol is dubious.

What Bakewell accomplished with this book, which is what she excels at, is to give those curious about Humanist belief a perspective centering history of Humanism. She also traces the many disparate ideas which form Humanism to those who originated the thoughts. It is an intellectually pleasing read, an entertaining tale of how Humanism came to be Humanism.

Much to Bakewell’s credit, she refused to dictate to the readers, she presented the historical facts and organized the history cogently so that those who are curious can read the history of development and evolution of Humanism and then she allows the reader to decide for themselves. Which is what a great author should do.

 

 

 

 

Sunday, November 26, 2023

Book Review-The Real Work: On the Mystery of Mastery By Adam Gopnik

I have been a longtime fan of Adam Gopnik’s writing and reporting for as long as I have been  a regular reader of the New Yorker magazine. His self-deprecating sense of humor and philosophical slant to his writing makes the stories not only enjoyable but also thought provoking. He has a style that is well thought out, erudite, and unforced.

I was drawn to his book as I have been interested in mastery for a long time, particularly the stories of people who have worked diligently towards achieving mastery; I am curious about their struggles with the process of achieving mastery, the lessons that they have learned about themselves, the nature of the process, and the demands working towards mastery places on their cognition, patience, and resilience.

The word mastery has been used and abused in the recent years because it has become the part of the lexicon for the quickie How-To business book industry, it has been metamorphosed into  a trite caricature by pretenders. Once the business book writing cabal find a hot popular topic such as mastery, the phrase is repeated ad infinitum by all the other members of the cabal. Every business book author ceaselessly repeats mastery as a mantra, but it is obvious that they are only interested in the shallow and superficial aspects of what it takes to achieve mastery. They tend to dig into the results of mastery rather than the hard intellectual and physical work necessary to achieve mastery. Since I am familiar with Gopnik’s previous work,  I knew that I could trust Gopnik to get beyond the superficial and trite to get deep into mastery.

The book occupies a much larger landscape than I expected. In fact, it is broader, was more introspective, and much more internal than what I had expected. I would say that the book both exceeded and expanded my expectations.  The coverage was more expansive while also was not fearful about being abstract.  

The book is, as expected,  full of philosophical digressions, which is why I love reading Gopnik. The subtitle of the book is the Mystery of Mastery. What is the mystery? Why is the idea of achieving mastery so mysterious?

The organization of the book interspersed the seven mysteries of mastery amongst the chapters devoted to the topics that Gopnik investigated and worked at to attain mastery. Obviously, he was not able to achieve mastery in all of the topics he investigated, but his willingness to jump into the process of learning and working towards mastery gave us a strong perspective on those things that he had to fight through to even get close to the edge of mastery.

The seven mysteries are:

·       Performance

·       Identity and Intention

·       Interiority

·       Meaning

·       Late Style

·       The Act Itself

·       And the Resolve.

Those chapters are illustrated through topics of investigation that Gopnik attempted:

·       Drawing

·       Magic

·       Driving

·       Baking

·       Boxing and Dancing

There is also an additional chapter that is devoted to a topic that is not about a skill or an action but a personal topic, which at first made me uneasy at first, but that was Gopnik’s intention. The personal topic is relieving. It took an act of courage for Gopnik to discuss this particular topic, and I appreciate his bravery and honesty. This chapter is a dive into gaining mastery over something that is personal and internal, it is a necessity for him rather than a choice that he made as a luxury.

Three topics drew my interest the most, although I learned something from each topic. Magic, Boxing, and Dancing were topics that I felt most comfortable with, for whatever reason. Magic stands out in this book because this seemed to be the center of why Gopnik dove in to investigate the topic of mastery. He delved into the history of magic and the mystique that had grown up through the years around performing magic. He also told stories about magicians, historical and present-day performers, digging into why they work so hard at their performances and what drew them into its grips. He investigates the motivations behind some of the biggest names in present day magic and tries to answer the question about their obsessions . The digressions into the history of magic was not really a digression per se, but a history lesson that gives context to the present-day magic scene and magicians.

Gopnik’s story telling ability pulled me into the details of the topics, no matter how mundane I felt the topics were, he has a way of looking at the different facets of the most normal and ordinary tasks and giving insight. The interspersing of the Mysteries of Mastery chapters perfectly complemented the stories and gave meaning to the stories and framed the mysteries through the context of each topic.

I have made a list of the mysteries in my Commonplace book to contemplate and to force myself to think in those terms. Perhaps I am making more out of Gopnik’s views of  mastery, yet I am willing to wager that I am not overthinking his mysteries. Regardless, I will be stimulated and entertained.

 

 

Saturday, November 25, 2023

Volleyball Coaching Life-Selection Process for the NCAA Tournament

Everyone becomes expert prognosticators when it comes to guessing which teams will make it into the 64 teams selected for the 2023 NCAA tournament. I am but one of many.

Below is the selection process from what I can recall off the top of my head. Some of the knowledge was related to me quite a few years ago by someone who was the chair of the selection committee that particular year. The details may have changed over the years, but I believe the process is the same. I apologize for any errors.

The selection committee must follow a process to figure out the final 64, the process imposes significant constraints on the decisions.

·       Only 16 teams are seeded instead of all 64 teams. I had hoped that they would start seeding more teams after they seeded all 48 teams during COVID, but they didn’t.

·       The top 16 teams have the option to host the first two rounds, the finals are in Tampa Bay and the regional finals, the third and fourth rounds, had been selected at the time as the finals. The right to host is subject to the NCAA’s guidelines on hosting regarding the quality of the facilities etc. Lockers for all the teams, lockers for the officials, etc. A top 16 seed could choose to not host, but that is crazy talk or they had constraints that they couldn’t overcome.

·       There are 32 automatic qualifiers, conference champions who have won the right to represent their conferences. The top teams in the field who won their conference also counts as the automatic qualifier for the conference.

·       The other teams are considered at large bids.

The RPI is the starting basis of the discussions. RPI is very controversial, and it proven to be not indicative of the strength of the teams since it purely depends on numerical data and it is an average of numerous factors. From Wikipedia:

The rating percentage index, commonly known as the RPI, is a quantity used to rank sports teams based upon a team's wins and losses and its strength of schedule. It is one of the sports rating systems by which NCAA basketballbaseballsoftballhockeysoccerlacrosse, and volleyball teams are ranked. This system was in use from 1981 through 2018 to aid in the selecting and seeding of teams appearing in the NCAA Division I men's basketball tournament as well as in the women's tournament from its inception in 1982 through 2020.

In its current formulation, the index comprises a team's winning percentage (25%), its opponents' winning percentage (50%), and the winning percentage of those opponents' opponents (25%). The opponents' winning percentage and the winning percentage of those opponents' opponents both comprise the strength of schedule (SOS). Thus, the SOS accounts for 75% of the RPI calculation and is 2/3 its opponents' winning percentage and 1/3 its opponents' opponents' winning percentages.

No opinions, no eye tests, strict numbers. Note that the AVCA coaches’ poll is not the starting point, nor is it ever used as reference. The committee does have access to the records of the NCAA regional coaches committee, these committees meet weekly during the season to discuss the teams in that region. This keeps the selection committee up to date on each of the top teams in the region during the season. This is one of the intangible factors that affects the discussions behind closed doors. These are coaches who volunteer their time to give important opinions on the top teams. Since this information does not affect the Coaches polls, they have little to gain personally.

The committee started the selection process Thanksgiving week, I would hazard to guess that they are meeting on Thanksgiving Day too, but I am not sure about that.

The first step is to break the field into four blocks of four. They take the first team on the list and compare their body of work to the second team’s body of work. The term body of work is important and often used in discussions because they are looking at the team’s accomplishment holistically, within the season.

The committee has available to them all the NCAA statistics as well as videos.

·       Head-to-head.

·       Record against top 50.

·       Record against top 25

·       Record against top 10.

·       Significant good wins, against teams ahead of them.

·       Significant bad losses, against teams below them.

·       Set scores, point differentials for good wins and bad losses are also available.

·       Lineups for any matches.

·       Record in the final ten matches of the season.

·       Parenthetically, PABLO was being considered to be used in the selection process, since PABLO was calculated based on predicting the outcome of a head-to-head meeting between two teams, I know the author of PABLO was adjusting the calculations to meet the selection committee’s requirement. I don’t know what became of the attempt to diversify the data set.

Note that the teams’ records that were already baked into the RPI are also included in the statistics used when the committee goes into debating the relative merits of their body of work. At the end of the debate, they decide whether to keep the same order or flip the order of the two teams. The same process goes through all the teams in the four-team block. Then they move to the next block of four, but they take the fifth team and compare their body of work with that of the fourth team, the last team on the first block, to decide on whether to keep the same order or flip. This goes on for all the 16 seeds. They will of course compare the body of work of the 17th team on the RPI and compare them to the last seeded team.

Since they don’t seed all 64 teams, they remove the automatic qualifiers who are not seeded already but are automatically included in the field. They work on the lowest RPI ranked at-large teams to include in the tournament by using the same process. The numbers of the last teams to be considered are different every season. The reason has to do with the way the automatic qualifiers resolve itself and whether the regular season champion or another team won the conference tournament — if they played a conference tournament.  This is why the announcers draw attention to the RPI.

The committee goes deep into the at-large teams, hedging their bets and giving themselves a good selection of backups. This is where the last four in and out come from.

Some good things to keep in mind:

·       While the RPI is the basis of the initial ranking, there are many ways to improve upon where the team ends up in RPI. This is why many coaches opt to schedule tough, reasoning that the 75% of the RPI that is dependent on the Strength Of Schedule (SOS) — opponents’ winning percentage and its opponents' opponents' winning percentages. Teams that are in a weak conference gets hurt by their conference because those wins does nothing to their SOS.

·       There are indirect ways to move up from your RPI, having good upset wins and avoiding bad upset loses.

·       The team’s record in the last 10 matches can be critical for some teams, it inserts a hot team into the tournament, all else being equal, to introduce that potential upset factor.

·       The committee does not look at all the teams from a macro level. As with the NCAA basketball selection, many will fault the selection for some strong early matchups that would be better suited, i.e. more competitive, for the later rounds, but I have to believe that the committee would want to have those later round competitive matches if they had a preference. They are following the rules dictated by the NCAA’s.

The same meetings are held using the same process by the AVCA awards committee to decide on the COY, POY, and the AA teams. The AA teams are further broken down by positions.

This is my favorite part of the season, I hope it is yours too. Enjoy.

 

Sunday, October 15, 2023

Book Review-Proust and the Squid By Maryanne Wolf

I read about this book some time ago. I didn't think it would be of interest to me at the time, but I bought the book anyways, as the topic piqued my curiosity enough so that I figured I might be interested later. True to form, I gained interest in the subject over time because of my interest in cognitive sciences. As I became more interested in how our brain learns, I came to appreciate the complexities of how human do what we do, which led to my interest in reading and this book, as reading is one of the most complicated skill to acquire.

Parenthetically, I was also reading Mortimer Adlers’ How to Read a Book (Adler, 1972) and Alberto Manguel’s  A History of Reading (Manguel, 1996) in parallel with reading Proust and the Squid. The three books covered the topic of reading from differing vantage points, so they complemented each other well in my mind, giving me great perspective.  I am now reading Marianne Wolf's second book Reader, Come Home (Wolf, 2018) to follow up on her work. The idea was to practice what Adler calls synoptic reading; this is to train myself to think deeply as Wolf talks about, because our mind is evolving as we  think deeply as we read broadly. Indeed, reading multiple books on the same topic has been not just enjoyable but also very good training.

Returning to this book, Proust and the Squid is split into three parts. Part One describes how our civilization developed language; a critical point that Wolf makes clear is that human brains  were never designed to read. The ability to read evolved as our brain adapted to synthesize many different parts of our neuro structure to integrate invented written language into our cognition. We learned to use the parts that had other principal functions that are necessary for survival and recruiting the  disparate parts of the brain for the purpose of becoming better at reading and to gain better understanding of what we read and write. We were able to take the original functions of those parts of the brain and synthesize them into developing coherent systems of writing and reading.  This was an extremely clever and significant development in our evolution; different cultures developed these abilities independently, as the different cultures were isolated from one another, which was surprising and serves to give ample proof to the concept of neuroplasticity.  A sampling of different languages shows that some are pictographic in nature, others are based upon sounds, and other languages are based on assigning meaning to the writing rather than just be based on the sound or appearance, which demonstrates how powerful our brain is in adapting to differing cultural needs. This section naturally leads into Parts Two of the book.

Part Two is a tutorial on how the brain learned to read over time. This was the main reason for my interest in reading the book. Chapters Four through Six told the story of how reading developed throughout history and how the brain developed and evolved into the reading and thinking machine. The key point that Wolf drummed into the reader as she beautifully told the story of reading is the fact that as we read more, the better we can think; and the better we are able to think, the better we are able to read and the more we can understand through reading. A positive feedback loop between reading and thinking. abilities. Yet another important point within the Part 2 is that we are shaped by what we read and how we read; which also affects how well we can understand and analyze what we read, a symbiotic relationship.

Part 3, on dyslexia — what happens when the reading brain of some people does not work the same way for the rest of us — was the part of the book that I was the least enthusiastic about tackling. Dyslexia and reading problems were never on my radar, it never piqued my interest, yet I found this part of the book,  Chapters seven and eight, fascinating, it is almost my favorite part of this book.

I had never studied dyslexia, how the brain  deviated from its usual reading process when people are dyslexic. I had thought of it as being somewhat mysterious, being an electrical engineer,  I always thought in terms of a short circuit somewhere. Part 3 of this book not only delineated all of the results from the theories and studies about dyslexia, it also reflected back on Part 2 of the book: how we learned how to read. The referral to Part 2 made Part 3 understandable, and Part 3 reinforced the narrative that Part 2 told. The two parts created a cohesive picture. I could not help but be riveted.

Chapter 9 is the conclusion chapter, which is where the author explains herself: the reasons for writing the previous eight chapters. She lays out the specific issues that raised alarms for her as a researcher in psychology and cognition, as well as a public intellectual that is alarmed about where our  society and culture is heading as far as reading and writing are concerned.

The first eight chapters set the stage by telling the reader the story of how we came to be readers and writers.  The final chapter interlaces the major themes and details of the fist eight into a well-knit argument, which served to be a warning about where our reading evolution is headed and by implication, where our ability to think is headed.

This book changed my mind about many things;  I learned all that I could handle about how closely coupled thinking and reading are and the extrapolation of that coupling could lead us in the future if our reading skills continues undisturbed down this present path.

I am just a very interested amateur in the neurosciences, and this book allowed me to take a giant  leap forward in my knowledge and made me more conversant in the research results and the structures of how our brains deal with reading, at least well enough to become a very committed amateur to learning more. I now have a scaffolding to hang onto as I continue to explore this area of study. Which is why I decided to read Readers Come Home.

Go and read this book. It is very important and it is delightful reading.

1.     References

Adler, M. (1972). How to Read Science and Mathematics. In C. V. Mortimer Adler, How To Read A Book (pp. 255-269). New York: Touchstone, Simon and Schuster.

Manguel, A. (1996). A History of Reading. New York City: Penguin Putnam Inc.

Wolf, M. (2018). Reader, Come Home: The Reading Brain in a Digital World. NYC: Harper Collins Books.

 

 

 

Saturday, September 9, 2023

Ruminations-On the Hoopla Over a Jewish Deli

Ever since All the Best Deli opened its doors, the discussions on the restaurant groups have been vociferous and partisan. Some of it have even become nasty. I suppose that is a reflection of the times that we are in. Even accounting for our present culture’s proclivity for polarization, it is still very surprising from the usually genteel people of the Midwest.

I had seen this report about Steingold’s Deli in Chicago. (https://abc7chicago.com/jewish-deli-chicago-near-me-steingolds/13303768/) and it occurred to me that one of the contributing reasons is the expectations that the general public has when they speak of delis. There is a huge difference between deli’s and Jewish deli’s as the reporter tried to explain in the video. It never occurred to me because as I had lived in cities such as Atlanta, St. Louis, and Ann Arbor, there were Jewish deli’s in those cities. It didn’t bother me that there wasn’t a Jewish deli in Dayton, although I did miss my latke’s, matzo ball, kreplach, and knishes. I also missed the meats: briskets, corned beef, and pastrami that did not come with the Boar’s Head label stamped on the packaging.

Not all delis are the same. Just as not all places who make sandwiches call themselves delis, any place that calls themselves delis are not automatically Jewish deli’s. The difference in price can be attributed to the difference between a place that calls itself a deli and one that calls itself a Jewish deli, it is not apples to apples.

A Jewish deli is a place where the meats are prepared by the establishment, come out of the tables steaming hot, and are then sliced for each order. The bread must be Jewish rye, baked in small batches, unless otherwise requested. There are minimal accoutrements but the menu also has plenty of other Jewish cuisine delicacies to fill my needs.

I need to declare at the outset that I don’t own stock in the restaurant, I don’t work there, nor am I related to anyone who owns of works there.  They don’t pay me for endorsements either. In fact, I have only been once, and I was quite a happy customer. I intend to go back again to sample the menu items that I had not sampled.

Being the curious sort that I am, I did a bit of Googling to see whether there are real Jewish delis in the surrounding area. The only place that I would call a Jewish deli is Shapiro’s in Indianapolis, this is where I would resort to when I needed my Jewish deli yen met. Matzo ball soup, an order of latkes, and big old sandwich on rye bread, the meat being the only variable.

I am unfamiliar with the food scene in Cincinnati and Columbus but in my cursory search, I found Izzy’s (Cincinnati) and Katzinger’s (Columbus)

I looked at the menu of All The Best Deli  and picked out some quintessential staples of a Jewish deli. I then tried to compile a table comparing Jewish deli’s in the other cities. As it turned out, Izzy’s and Katzingers offered less than half of what All The Best Deli offers, so I took them off the comparison.

I selected Steingold’s in Chicago, Shapiro’s in Indianapolis, and Katz’s Deli in New York City, the iconic representative of a proper Jewish deli in my humble opinion. I was able to compare the prices for five menu items that are offered by both All The Best Deli and Steingold’s; seven menu items that are offered by both All The Best Deli and Shapiro’s; eight menu items that are offered by both All The Best Deli and Katz’s. I could not accurately judge the portion size comparisons between the different restaurants, so that is something for people to continue to kvetch about.

Some salient points, I had selected a dozen items from All The Best, three were dropped because they weren’t offered by the majority of the Jewish deli’s I selected; I was down to nine menu items. The restaurants in the Midwest had mostly comparable prices. Katz’s prices were astronomical as compared to the midwestern restaurants.  To put things into better perspective I looked up the cost-of-living comparisons between Dayton to NYC and adjusted the Katz’s prices to bring it closer to a Dayton equivalence. For your information, the cost of Living in NYC is 43% more than Dayton. The farthest right column shows the adjusted price. The comparison between the adjusted Katz’s price and as compared to the All The Best menu price, they are not all that different.  

One last variable in the menu pricing is that All The Best proudly proclaims that they source all of their foods from the best Jewish deli’s around the country since they wanted the best and most authentic Jewish food products available (https://allthebestdeli.com/our-story/ ), which means that they have to pay massive shipping costs to schlep all the food regularly from wherever the other deli’s are and get them to Dayton, which adds to the cost of doing business.

Here is to the wishful thinking that the malevolent tones of the kvetchers will ameliorate somewhat if they read this. But I highly doubt that.

 



 

Saturday, August 26, 2023

Book Review-How to Read a Book: The Classic Guide to Intelligent Reading By Mortimer J. Adler and Charles Van Doren

I bought this book many years ago with the intention to carefully absorb the wisdom of Adler and Van Doren. I had made several attempts, mainly picking and choosing sections of the book as it suited my purpose at that point in time. Presently, having some time on my hands, I tackled this tome this Summer.

It is not a monstrously long book, about 350 pages long, not including the appendices and end notes. The intimidating aspect of the book has everything to do with the reputation of the authors, as they are what was once known as public intellectuals. Mortimer Adler and Charles Van Doren are renowned philosophers whose credentials are publicly celebrated. I knew the contents would be densely packed, but I also knew that as careful and excellent writers, they sought to communicate first, rather than to impress.

I was immediately taken into the scholarly environment that they had set out to convey at the beginning. Chapter one simply and clearly lays out their intent and sets the reader’s expectations at a high level. This opening chapter gives the readers an exact outline of what they seek to convey. Not a word was wasted, not a false impression created.

The book is well structured and lays out an excellent roadmap for the reader to navigate. Every argument is carefully enumerated and easily tracked. There are four parts to the book, split into twenty-one chapters. Part III of the book, chapters thirteen to nineteen, is devoted to specific topics: math and sciences, history, philosophy, social sciences, et. al. and how to read these chapters analytically. I am saving Part III for later. My strategy for reading Part III is  to find the keys and identify the propositions in the manner that the authors had laid put in Part II, thus using the lessons learned from the book itself.

The authors divided reading into four levels: elementary, inspectional, analytical, and finally synoptical. Most of us had gone through the elementary level as we learned to read. Inspectional reading is something that some of us have learned to do if our chosen field of study involved massive amounts of reading, or if we are bibliophiles and consume massive amounts of books. Inspectional reading is what we do when we are sifting through the reading material that is laid out in front of us, be they for enjoyment, personal acquisition of knowledge, or for our vocations. It gives us a chance to sort through the reading material, identify those material that are pertinent to our interests, and allows us to formulate a reading strategy for each book. Elementary and inspectional reading is covered in Part I of the book.

Part II is devoted to the topic of analytical reading. Analytical reading is the type of reading that some of us do regularly. It is how we should be reading so that the readers are able to identify the  topic that the author is proposing and identify the arguments that the authors are making.  It is also how the readers should be reading to best ascertain our own opinions and critical judgements about the author’s intent. Analytical reading is the key to determining whether the authors was successful in communicating their key points through their propositions, whether their arguments are cogent and rational, and whether they are successful in laying out a complete vision of their subject. The authors devoted a significant number of pages to teach the readers on how to judge the author’s viewpoints and the effectiveness of the arguments without resorting to emotional reactions or calling upon the one’s habit to react impulsively. This is a perspicacious, prescriptive, and anticipatory decision, as most average readers will reflexively react procedurally without exercising their critical thinking ability and calling upon their analytical abilities.

Part IV is devoted to synoptical reading, a kind of reading that some people do, but they rarely call it synoptical reading. It is the practice of reading numerous books on the same subject. The motivation for the reader is to gain understanding of the subject, which may encompass numerous different books and authors,  rather than assuming that the knowledge resides in just a single book. The focus is on the subject rather than on a designated book. The tools that are taught in Part II for analytical reading are expanded and generalize as befitting the broad scope of reading.

My first reaction after I started reading the book was that I wish that I had read this book earlier in my life, it would have prevented the meandering path that my attention and my curiosity had been taken during my research life. Fortunately, all is not lost because much of what was explicitly stated in the book, particularly in Part II, had somehow been integrated into my reading habits implicitly through my own reading and analysis experiences. There were numerous moments of recognition and proud realization as I read through this book, although there were also many forehead slapping moments of enlightenment. I also started to consciously implement the lessons that I had learned on the fly as I was reading. The result is that my reading acuity has improved discernably as I learned in real time.

This improvement was particularly significant as I was reading synoptically. The insights from the book helped me to avoid going into a death spiral of confusion as I tried to read synoptically on a topics by reading different books and authors. This is the trap that a reader will often fall into when reading synoptically as the focus needs to necessarily be on the topic rather then a book and author. My synoptic reading skill has improved markedly even though I was trying to change my reading habits in real time as I read the book.

While the nature of the writing in this book is dense with details and was as challenging as I had expected from the authors, they had made their structure transparent to the reader to help ease the reader’s way through the material. They created hooks in the text for the reader to hang on to so that the readers can follow the prescribed methodology as the reader develops their reading strategy. Even as I became lost, I was able to recover the train of the argument simply and expediently because of the clarity of the writing and book structure.

Indeed, this book was as I had expected, delivering even more than as promised. While I still have a touch of regret about my own procrastination before reading this book, I am glad I finally had a chance to settle in and immerse myself.

I am convinced that a reader must be ready for the book, any book. I am glad I proved to be  ready now, after many years of dawdling. I still wonder if my comprehension of what I had read in the interim would have helped, I imagine that I could have gleaned more from all my readings if I could have absorbed the lessons of this book earlier in my life. My regret is assuaged by the fact that I might not have had the maturity to handle the lessons in a previous time and place. Indeed, it is better late than never.

I recommend this to all serious readers who are in search of knowledge

Saturday, August 19, 2023

Learning and Teaching-Cognitive Load Theory Optimizing Intrinsic Load Part 4

In Part 1 on Cognitive Load Theory (https://polymathtobe.blogspot.com/2023/02/learning-and-teaching-cognitive-load.html) , the framework of WHAT Cognitive Load Theory is was laid out in principle, following  Oliver Lovell’s book Cognitive Load Theory In Action on the subject (Lovell 2020).

Part 2 is on how teachers can minimize extrinsic load on the learner through honing their  presentation. (https://polymathtobe.blogspot.com/2023/04/learning-and-teaching-cognitive-load.html)

Part 3 is on how teachers can minimize the extrinsic load on the learner through structuring their practices and lessons. (https://polymathtobe.blogspot.com/2023/05/learning-and-teaching-cognitive-load.html)

This article roughly follows Oliver Lovell’s book in examining how the teacher, coach, and learner can apply  Cognitive Load Theory to minimize the extrinsic loading on the working memory and then learn to optimize the intrinsic load in the working memory.

The definitions of intrinsic and extrinsic loads are defined again for ease of reference.

The extrinsic cognitive loads are:

·       A part of  the manner and structure of how the information is conveyed to the learners.

·       Disruptive to the learning task because it distracts the learner from learning by occupying valuable working memory space.

Whereas the intrinsic cognitive loads are those that are critical to learning whatever it is that we need to learn. They are:

·       Part of the nature of the information that we are learning.

·       Core learning.

·       Information that we WANT the learner to have in their working memory.

The critical limitation is that the working memory has a finite capacity; that is, the intrinsic and the extrinsic loads are vying for the same finite resource. The emphasis is placed on minimizing the extrinsic load; that is, to offload unnecessary extrinsic cognitive load, to make space in the working memory before optimizing the intrinsic loads.

Note that even though Lovell’s book is relatively short, he presents quite a bit of results and information gained from the studies that make definitive arguments and gives excellent implementation examples, so it is worthwhile to read through the book.

Since I am coming from two familiar but different points of view: teaching at a university level and coaching, I will try to illustrate the points by giving simple examples from both milieus.

The defining difference between teaching and coaching is that teaching can be effected over a longer time frame. Learners in the academic milieu can take their time in building up the learning scaffolds because the academic learner does not need to implement the material immediately, there is time to spend on repeatedly going over the material, digging into the granularity as well as examining the broad scope of the topics. In sports coaching, the learner is expected to learn to act and react instantaneously to new situations which are never identical to the contrived practice situations, this increases extraneous extrinsic load on their working memory as they struggle to learn.

Optimizing Intrinsic Load

The language used to describe the necessary actions for intrinsic loads and extrinsic loads are different. Intrinsic loads need to be optimized, which means that the intrinsic loads can be maximized, minimized, or remain the same.

Optimizing the intrinsic load is the key to successful learning, the difference in wording for intrinsic and extrinsic loading is intentional and comes because of the effect that intrinsic and extrinsic load has on the working memory. The extrinsic loads must be minimized because it is extraneous and impedes learning. The goal is to  devote the maximum of the available working memory to intrinsic loading, it is the loading that facilitates maximum learning. Even as the extrinsic load is minimized, the amount of intrinsic loading placed on the learner could still overwhelm the learner’s working memory.

Intrinsic loads need to be optimized, i.e., adjusted to accommodate the learner’s learning capacity. If the learner can handle more intrinsic load, then the teacher must fill in the void in the learner’s working memory. If the learner is overloaded with intrinsic loads, then the teacher must simplify and remove some of the intrinsic loads to enable the learner to learn effectively. The next question is how do you know when to add and when to simplify? As with all things human, it depends on the person. Each learner is different, and their learning experience needs to be accommodated.  Slightly overloading the learner’s working memory may be beneficial because stressing the learner can sometimes accelerate their adaptation to the intrinsic load and allows them to automatically chunk the topics that is loading them down. At the same time, overstressing the working memory might diminish their ability to learn. There are many factors to this: whether the learner is a novice or an expert, the amount of related knowledge resident in the learner’s long-term memory which can be leveraged to associate with the new knowledge, the amount of time and the amount of exposure the learners are allowed to the knowledge. The complexity and difficulties associated with the topic. These facts makes teaching to a large number of learners challenging, but not impossible.

The overall aim of teaching is not just to teach the fundamental of the topic: the tools of the craft; the overall aim of teaching is to teach how to use the tools effectively and efficiently. Not just the experiential aspect of learning: the how’s, why’s, what if’s; but also, the ability to extrapolate and using their knowledge and reasoning abilities to adapt, improvise, and overcome.

As stated before, there are two key reasons for optimizing the intrinsic loads: to adjust the level of intrinsic load placed on the working memory, which eases the learning process for the learner without exceeding the finite working memory;  and also using the learner’s working memory to its fullest capability without exceeding the capacity.  

The former requires that the teacher adapt the material to the different learning abilities of the learner. The latter requires that the teacher structure the learning so that the learner can fully leverage their finite working memory resource, preventing the learner’s attention from wandering. Not too much, not too little, but just enough; optimize rather than just maximize.

Expertise-Reversal Effect

The Expertise Reversal Effect compounds the complexity of the strategies for optimizing intrinsic load.

The expertise-reversal effect suggests that learners need differing amounts of support depending upon their level of expertise.

This is explained by Guadagnoli and Lee in their paper (Guadagnoli 2004). In short, those learners who are learning new skills and concept without any previous experience are more likely to be lost and confused while learning the basics because their long-term memory is not equipped with existing tangible memories to retrieve which would aid in learning and can be leveraged to create new knowledge for the learner. 

The expert-reversal effect has significant impact on how the teacher needs to approach and implement their optimization strategy. That is, they need to carefully consider the level of the learners they are trying to reach and adjust their strategy.

Teaching

In the context of teaching engineering, the expert-reversal effect implies that :”…worked examples are better for novices, and problem solving is better for experts…”, i.e. the novices needs to be led through the example in discrete steps, and shown the reasoning for each step; while the experts can be given the problem to solve directly.

It can mean that the teacher must be judicious while choosing when to switch between solving examples and assigning problems. It is better to nudge the novice learners a bit more aggressively to solve problems rather than boring them with too many of the same examples, the reverse effect is that the bored learners will tune out the teacher.

Coaching

In the coaching context, the expert-reversal effect means that the coach needs to build up the novice learners’ long-term memory with tangible experiences. Initial experiences are critical to the novice learner because that is the scaffolding for their future learning, not only must the initial experience be practical for the moment, but they must also be useful in the future.

This is asking for quite a bit. The expert-reversal effect will be invoked later in the article to illustrate the impact of the expert-reversal effect has on the decisions for the teaching strategies chosen by the coaches.

A note on the term “tangible experiences”. There has been discussion about the topic of specificity. Some literalists insist that unless the experience is completely in the same domain and context, that experience is not pertinent. In the neurological explanation from Scott Grafton’s Physical Intelligence (Grafton 2020), in which I am an amateur, the neurons don’t know that they are firing for something completely different from the purpose that it had initially acquired the “muscle synergy” and the “basis set”. The neurons are firing because it has that experience to reuse in its “efference model”.

In other words, the neurons will fire for the arm motion of throwing a ball, even if the movement required is not throwing the ball. The bottom line is that the accrual of all motor skills could potentially be beneficial sometime, if not specifically for a domain or context.

Pre-teaching

“Pre-teaching is delivering a portion of the content before the main lesson, and reinforcing it through revision over time, can reduce the intrinsic load experienced by the learner when they attempt the final, complete task.” (Lovell 2020)

Pre-teaching focuses the learner’s attention on the pre-taught material and places it in the working memory of the learner prior to the formal instruction. It gives a basis for the learner to use for later learning.

Teaching

·       Teacher can foreshadow by introducing the coming topics before diving into the details.

·       Teachers can also expose the learners to the big picture of the elements of the topics prior to diving into the subtopics, giving them a framework to place each topic.

Coaching

·       Coaches can introduce advanced skills in practice, partly as incentives, and partly to expose the learners to the future of their own athletic abilities.

Part-Whole or Whole-Part

Part-whole means building constituent skills and knowledge before putting is all together. Whole -part requires providing a general overview first, followed by more focused practice of individual segments. (Lovell 2020)

On the surface, the difference between part-whole and whole-part may seem like rhetoric but the decisions have a significant impact on how well the learner can use their working memory to optimize the intrinsic load, i.e., how well the learner learns.

Part-whole partitions a topic into constituent parts, teaching the constituent parts in some imposed sequence, and reintegrating the constituent parts into the whole. Whole-part teaches the whole topic first and selectively isolate on the parts through partitioning and emphasizing the parts as the whole topic is taught.

There are two questions to be asked before making the decision: one is about the experience and maturity of the learners we are trying to reach; the other is about the complexity of the topic that is being taught. The expert-reversal effect tells us that novices do not have the fundamental tools or experiences to effectively integrate the whole topic into their intrinsic load, while the experts had already effectively integrated a significant portion of the topic into their long term memory. It seems obvious that the part-whole approach is best for the novice and the whole-part is best for the expert, because the expert can leverage their previous tools and experiences to create new knowledge. I would say that the whole-part becomes more beneficial to the learner as they progress from zero experience to expertise. The usual mistake is to maintain the part-whole paradigm too often.

Yet another qualifying criterion that need to be considered is whether the proposed partition of the topic is cognitively logical in isolation; that is, whether the parts can be presented logically without taking into consideration the cross-coupling effects that may be inherent in the topic. If so, then taking a part-whole approach is better for the novice because doing so would decrease the intrinsic loading of the learner to a manageable level. If the individual partitioned parts do not make sense in isolation, then the whole-part approach needs to be implemented. One major caveat is that the act of partitioning a topic needs to be done carefully because the couplings may not be obvious. It is the not-knowing-what-one-does-not-know effect.

Part-Whole

The part-whole approach is to start simple and build complexity as the learner adjusts to the increased load, the expense is that the holistic view is ignored until later in the learning process.

The arguments for the part-whole approach are:

·       “The initial presentation of the part tasks helps consolidate procedures or rules, which can be applied to the whole task at a later stage.” (Lovell 2020)

·       If the topic that is taught is complex, the complexity can overload the learner’s working memory so that the integration of the total task serves to the detriment of learning.

The thrust of the part-whole presentation approach is that the lesson must meet the level of the learner instead of the teacher. Presenting the whole skill at the beginning of the learning process may overwhelm the novice’s working memory.

Lovell lists a few techniques that can be deployed to implement the part-whole approach.

·       Chain forward-Forward chaining presents the partitioned parts of the skills in the chronological order in which they appear in the total skill.

·       Chain backward-Backward chaining is just the reverse, it presents the partitioned parts of the skills in backwards chronological order, starting from the ending.

·       Snowball-Snowballing is a variation on the forward and backward chaining. The idea is to perform a newly introduced partitioned parts of the skills with the previously presented partitioned parts of the skills in conjunction. As the new partitioned part of the skills is added to the repertoire, all the previous taught skills are integrated into the practice. This is to facilitate the learner to Chunk the partitioned parts of the skills together. Chunking is explained later in this article.

One note. Practicing partitioned parts of the skill may not resemble the whole skill in the least. The teacher’s feedback needs to reflect that fact, to not compare apples to oranges. The ultimate goal is to successfully execute the whole skill, not the partitioned parts of skills.

Whole-Part

The whole-part approach is to present the whole topic first and then simplify the complex whole skill as the learner’s learning progress demands to help the learner to absorb the simplified topic.

The argument for the whole-part approach is:

·       “For complex motor tasks and many professional real-life tasks, it is essential that the learner understand and learn the relevant interactions and coordination between the various subtasks. By learning the subtasks in isolation, these interactions may be missed.” (Lovell 2020)

·       Generally, presenting the whole skill first seem to make more sense because we want the learner to be exposed to the holistic view, this way the linkage and coupling of the parts can be demonstrated and illustrated. This helps the learner to picture the whole topic and can allow them to anticipate.

Lovell also lists a few techniques that can be deployed to implement the whole-part  approach.

·       Simplifying conditions-Simplifying the conditions mean that while the whole skill is being practiced and learned, the conditions under which the practices are conducted are simplified to ease the intrinsic loading on the learner.  Rather than focusing on the entire skill under real conditions, take away the real constraints and open up the degrees of freedom under which the skills are being practiced.

·       Manipulating the emphasis-By minimizing the total number of emphases of the skill, just have the learner focus on the parts of the whole skill that are giving them problems. This serves to unload some of the working memory so that the learner is not overwhelmed.

·       Introducing variations-This technique does not minimize the intrinsic loading as the previous techniques. Indeed, adding variations to a practice increases the intrinsic loading on the learners, it increases the intrinsic load. So why is this an effective technique? Introducing variations stresses the learnings working memory exposing them to tasks that are not in their repertory, it forces them to struggle with the complexity of performing the skill with increased variation.  The learner is forced to develop new neuronal pathways which create new working memory which is based on existing experience. These newly created neuronal pathways will be integrated into new long-term memory. One note of caution, the teacher needs to be perspicacious about how the learner reacts to the variation. They could easily overload their working memory. There is a fuzzy limit to how much overloading of the working memory with intrinsic serves to force the learner to adapt to the challenge or as an unintended consequence, completely overwhelming the learner. I am of the belief that most learners are often more resilient than the teacher believes. The best option is to just experiment.

Teaching

·       Teaching is usually taught as a part-whole exercise with the teacher walking through examples step by step.

·       The students will often be ahead of the examples and are able to anticipate the next step.

·       Backward chaining and snowballing would make an interesting exercise in helping the learner learn how to anticipate and connecting the parts.

·       In teaching the qualitative topics, the whole-part approach is taken by introducing the topic from a macro point of view, giving the student the linkages, which connect the different topics and fields within a broad topic. This way the learner can use the relationships taught holistically to anticipate and extrapolate into future topics.

·       A particular problem is that teachers tend to keep the learners in the part-whole realm for too long. Repeatedly giving the learners the same examples and problems to solve rather than giving the learners opportunities to extemporize on the partitioned parts of the skills that they have learned. Problem solving should be about the tool and abilities to make connections between each step.

Coaching

·       For absolute novices, a part-whole approach is best to not overwhelm them cognitively and over stress them emotionally. The frustration from being unable to grasp the entirety of a skill is a showstopper for many learners.

·       Coaches also tend to keep the learners in the part-whole realm for too long. Having the players drilling on the same part of the overall skill to perfection before allowing the players to extemporize and to learn how to solve problems on the court or pitch. Problem solving should be about the tool and abilities to make connections between each step. This problem is especially acute when playing a sport because the time necessary for problem solving and decision making is miniscule.

·       Start with part-whole for novices and proceed to whole-part as soon as possible. There is no room in sports training for perfection.

General Practices

Some general practices that are often recommended are listed here to demonstrate how the Cognitive Load Theory is applied.  These practices are particularly useful for optimizing the intrinsic loading.

Chunking

According Lemov’s The Coaches Guide to Teaching (Lemov 2020): Experts processes more information than novices because they process information in chunks.  This is a key goal of teaching: to induce the learner to chunk their information — knowledge and experiences — together so that the working memory capacity is less laden when the chunks are recalled because chunks are multiple pieces of information chunked together. According to Lemov. The practice of chunking is domain and context specific; that is, the chunks will most likely be useless when taken to a different domain and placed in a different context. Which by the way, contradicts the adage: the game teaches the game. The game teaches the game if and only if the learner understands the domain and context of their knowledge and experiences, that understanding gives meaning to the chunks of  knowledge.

The techniques that are mentioned in both the part-whole and whole-part sections will all expose the learner to the logical chain that underlies the main skill. Chaining, snowballing, simplification, manipulating the emphasis, and introducing variations all link the partitioned parts of the skills into the whole skill. Snowballing is particularly effective in creating the circumstances under which the learner can link and associate the different parts of the skills into the whole skills.

The limiting factor for chunking is the number of knowledges the working memory can handle at once. Once again, the expert-reversal effect affects the total number of tasks a learner can handle without overloading the working memory. The rule of thumb was that the average human working memory can manage to focus on seven salient tasks, although I have read in various literature that experts — especially while under duress — can only focus on three to four salient   tasks at once. It may be that a novice may only be able to focus on  one or two tasks at once. Indeed, it is more pragmatic to empirically decide individually on how many tasks a person can focus on.

Retrieval Spacing and Interleaving

Retrieval, spacing, and Interleaving practices go hand in hand. I first read about the practice in Brown,  Roedinger, et, al. Making It Stick (Brown 2014) and then it was reinforced in Lemov (Lemov 2020).

The idea is to plan and schedule the practice to give the learners a chance to actively retrieve the memory of the skill as often as possible from the long-term memory. Constant retrieval of the memories from long term memory strengthens the memory and migrates the memory closer to the top of the stack in the long-term memory, helping to make the memory permanent.

Spacing works in combination with retrieval practice. By spacing segments of the same practice in time, allows the memories of the practice subject to fade from the working memory and when the same practice is re-initiated later, whether it is within the same dedicated time segment or not, the memories are retrieved.

Interleaving accomplishes both retrieval and spacing by practicing in cycles rather than in one continuous sequence. Rather than doing one drill or studying one subject for a given amount of time or for the accomplishment of a final goal, interleave the same drills or study period by doing them in cycles. Examples are shown below.

Unfortunately, there are no general rules of thumb regarding the number of times retrievals need to happen to make the knowledge permanent in long term memory. Nor is there a recommended time for spacing which is optimal to guarantee forgetting and retrieval, I had asked Prof. Brown in an email on this aspect, he told me there had not been any studies in that regard, it depends on the specific group of learners with each learner having a distinct timing and the complexity of the topic.

I have tried to make retrieval practices as prevalent and numerous as possible in both teaching and coaching.

Teaching

·       Instituting short term assessments such as quizzes at regular intervals to motivate the learners to retrieve prior knowledge. Each quiz is comprehensive, not just focusing on the topic of the week.

·       Question and answer periods within the recitation where the teacher cold call students on previously learned topics, giving the learners opportunities to actively retrieve previously learned knowledge.

·       All assessments are comprehensive.

·       Alluding to previously covered topics and integrating them into the new topics, creating connections and context for the old and the new topics.

·       Encouraging the learners to modify their study habits by committing to 20–25-minute blocks that are devoted to one topic, then taking a 5-minute break before moving to another topic. But the learner must return to the topic at least once before the studying session is over, to actively retrieve the knowledge.

o   Study subject 1 for 20-25 minutes. Take a 5-minute break.

o   Study subject 2 for 20-25 minutes. Take a 5-minute break.

o   Study subject 3 for 20-25 minutes. Take a 5-minute break.

o   Return to subject 1.

o   Return to subject 2.

o   Return to subject 3.

o   Repeat as convenient.

Coaching

·       Retrieval, spacing, and interleaving can be combined in effective practice planning. Rather planning on having numerous drills, each lasting until a performance or timed target is achieved.

o   Traditionally:

§  Drill 1, with a time and/or performance goal.

§  Drill 2, with a time and/or performance goal.

§  Drill 3, with a time and/or performance goal.

§  Drill 4, with a time and/or performance goal.

o   Using interleaving

§  Drill 1, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted to reduce the total time spent from the traditional way.

§  Drill 2, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted to reduce the total time spent from the traditional way.

§  Drill 3, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted to reduce the total time spent from the traditional way.

§  Drill 4, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted to reduce the total time spent from the traditional way.

§  Drill 1 again, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted and based on the time and performance goal from the first time through.

§  Drill 2 again , with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted and based on the time and performance goal from the first time through.

§  Drill 3 again, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted and based on the time and performance goal from the first time through.

§  Drill 4 again, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted and based on the time and performance goal from the first time through.

§  Drill 1 again, with a time and/or performance goal that is adjusted and based on the time and performance goal from the second time through.

§  And so on.

§  Note that the drill segments do not need to be in sequence, changing the order randomly is implementing the introduction of variations technique in the Whole-part paradigm. The key is to not put the same drills back-to-back, which defeats the purpose.

§  By judiciously changing the goals — time goals or performance goals — the intrinsic load is varied with each repetition of the drill, creating an elevating desirable difficulty to the intrinsic loading. Again, attention need to be paid to the learner’s response, to decide if the variation is overloading their working memory.

§  Interleaving, if properly practiced, can be more time effective than devoting a large block of time to each drill. Frequent changing of emphasis and physical requirements ensures that the learners are constantly being challenged and refreshed rather than being stuck in an interminable rut.

§  Depending on the performance goal selected, the learners may achieve the final desired goal through the escalating intermediate goals quicker than trying to achieve the desired goal at once.

§  Another element is to introduce a scrimmage or play segment in between the drills, allowing the learners to quickly incorporate the skills from the drills after the first cycle of the drill or through the sequence. This gives the teacher a chance to give them feedback on what they are missing and remind them of the purpose of the drills. All in time to go through the second cycle of the drill or drills. Repeat the scrimmage or play segment as desired.

Block and Random

Much like the part-whole versus whole-part discussion, the block and random debate has been controversial and going on indefinitely, especially in the sports context. While the vast majority of coaches agree that random practices more closely resemble reality, either in the classroom or on the sporting fields or courts, sometimes block training is necessary. Those instances are when the expert-reversal effect comes into play.

Going back to the arguments from before, block practices are necessary for those novices who need to integrate the basics through scaffolded repetitions. Random repetitions, while much more beneficial for anyone who is not a novice, imposes too much extrinsic load on the novice and will retard the initial learning. The problem is that most teachers will resort to block training intuitively. It is partly because they have been taught this way and because they want the learners to execute the skills immaculately prior to exposing them to reality. Learners are by and large more resilient and adaptive than teachers assume, which means that the optimal transition period to random training comes much sooner than the teachers estimate.

Summary

This article lays out several topics that I found personally convincing with regard to many topics that incorporates the ideas inherent in the Cognitive Load Theory. My extemporizing has digressed somewhat, but it encapsulates much of what I believe to be the foundation of my philosophy of teaching and coaching. It took a long way to get there.

Learning is not a game of perfect, it is a process that requires constant and consistent elevation of challenges to the learner, creating a process of desirable difficulties which stimulates their ability to integrate existing knowledge and experiences and to challenge their ability to create new personal solutions through problem solving and decision making.

I am convinced that the Cognitive Load Theory is the best model we have for understanding how the human learning process works. I have tried to examine the topic synoptically through reading several references, of course it is impossible to completely include every study and tome on the topic in my reading or to encapsulate the ideas in my mind. My working memory would be overloaded. 😊

I welcome any discussion with those experts in the areas of learning, skill acquisition, neuro, and cognitive science because the topic fascinates this dilettante.

References

Brown, Peter C. ,Roediger III, Henry L. , McDaniel,Mark A. Make It Stick: The Science of Successful Learning. Canbridge MA: Belknap Press, 2014.

Grafton, Scott. Physical Intelligence. New York: Pantheon Books, 2020.

Guadagnoli, Mark and Timothy D. Lee. "Challenge Point: a Framework for Conceptualizing the Effects of Various Practice Conditions in Motor Learning." Journal of Motor Behavior, June 2004: 212-224.

Lemov, Doug. The Coaches Guide to Teaching. Clearwater, FL: John Catt Educational Ltd., 2020.

Lovell, Oliver. Sweller's Cognitive Load Theory in Action. Melton: John Catt Educational Ltd, 2020.